Orientation: Understanding Multiple Regression Results
To understand patterns in the universe scientists often want to know how much a measured variable corresponds to another measured variable from the same cases. For example, if we want to know if people learn more words with each passing day, it would be nice to have the number of days old a person is and how many words the person can say—two measures for each person in the study. (To protect the anonymity of the measures we will give them pseudonyms X and Y. In writing actual research it is better to give variables names that are meaningful to the reader.).
If there is a general increase in one measure accompanying an increase in the other measure (e.g., more words with being more days old), there is a monotonic relationship between the variables. If it is basically proportional (e.g., 3 more words per day), then it is a linear relationship. (Notice that there could be a monotonic relationship that is a square or a log or other increasing but “curved” relationships. The linear analyses discussed here will not capture anything besides linear, so if the actual relationship function had a lot of non-linear components, they would not be analyzed well with linear methods. A sine wave has a lot of up and down linear components, but would average to 0. A circle has, technically, no linear components but there is a perfect non-linear relation between X and Y for a circle).
From elementary algebra you probably remember the formula for the slope of a line, which measures how steeply Y increases with increases in X:
For the two lines on the left, b is what the value of X is where the line hits the Y axis—the intercept. “m” represents the steepness of the slope. The red line shows that Y increases as X does, so the slope is positive and m > 0. The purple line (or “purpline” for short) shows a decrease in Y as X increases, so its m < 0. A line with m = 0 is flat—Y is constant and equals b. More important about a line with no slope: knowing what X is will give you no information whatsoever about what Y is.
Now here’s the thing – math is perfect. It is also fiction. It’s made up to be perfect. With a linear equation you can ALWAYS know exactly what Y will be if you are told X, or vice versa. (It’s like being followed around by somebody who ALWAYS says the punch lines.)
Real life, though, is messy. (Unless you are a very neat person who lives solely with other neat people, imagine a “house” or “apartment.” Does your actual home look like that, or is there some clutter in reality that you would have to erase to make it look like the home you put in a magazine? We scientists have to know how to wade through the clutter without throwing out the things that would make a house recognizable, e.g., not the kitchen sink.) In real life, nothing is measured perfectly, but we still have to figure out if there are patterns lurking beneath the clutter and dust and blurry glass despite imperfect measurement. We would like to be able to know how much Y might be if we learn X, and we also want to know how much to bet on that exact figure, or leave more latitude for imprecision.
In real life, we get dirt—specks of data that don’t have to line up (see left). However imperfect, can X give us a good guess about what Y is?
When we measure all the dirt specks for X and Y, then we have data. Just like the slope m in algebra, in statistics, the correlation coefficient r tells us how much (if at all) X and Y have a linear slope. If r is 0 (or close enough to it), there is no linear pattern to the relationship. If r is very close to 1.0 (and it can’t get bigger than that), that means a one unit increase in X will produce a one unit increase in Y, on average. Likewise, if r is very close to -1.0 (and it can’t get smaller than that), that means a one unit increase in X will produce a one unit decrease in Y, on average.
The correlation coefficient ignores the intercept or means. But if you want to know what that is and how strong the correlation is, you can get that too, using “regression.” (This does not mean turning into a baby so just keep paying attention!) Regression is a minimization technique for taking a bunch of observations of two variables (the eponymous X and Y) and estimating what the slope and intercept are. Because they are not perfect predictions, unlike algebra, they have different names than m and b: The intercept is the constant c which is estimated by the mean. The slope is known as beta (β). The minimization technique used to estimate β is to make the distance between each observed data point and the line –totaled all up—to be as small as possible.
Multiple regression just means that instead of having only one X variable, you have more than one predictor variable (plus the Y). You already know you could correlate X1 with Y, X2 with Y, X3 with Y and so forth. Multiple regression means you put all of the predictor variables you have in and let them fight it out for how much each X relates to Y when the others are in the ring. You get a different regression coefficient for each predictor variable (like β1 for X1, β2 for X2, etc.).
If the predictors are strongly correlated to each other, then putting more than one predictor in might not add more (new) information. So like if X1 and X2 are strongly correlated, then maybe only β1 will be non-zero because X2 doesn’t add much more information to predicting Y than X1 does.
All this is to help you be able to read not just graphs of results, but also tables of regression coefficients. Just like with correlation coefficients (that only consider 1 X and 1 Y alone), standardized regression coefficients can range from 1 to -1, and the more the value is away from 0, the steeper the slope or stronger the linear relation between X and Y. Just like with correlation coefficients and other inferential statistics, there is usually a p-value given (such as p < .05) that indicates the estimated chance that the observed relationship would be a measurement accident rather than a real relationship you can count on.
Standardization
Just like in linear algebra, a regression weight converts the units of the X to units of the Y. For example, if gather a bunch of dog’s weights and head heights, and measured them in pounds, we can be sure that the regression coefficient in inches would be 2.54 times the size of the regression coefficient in cm, because 1 inch = 2.54 cm. But with the same data, it would NOT be the case that, magically, the cm regression weight would show a stronger relationship than would the inches regression weight, just because it is a bigger number.
To get rid of this problem, we can standardize the regression weights by considering the units they are measured in and essentially dividing the unit out. What we do is we multiple the unstandardized coefficient by the ratio of the standard deviations (SD) of the outcome and the predictor variables:
β = B × SDy/SDx
Application:
Howard, Blumstein & Schwartz (1986) conducted a study of 320 adult couples, in which they asked each partner separately to rate how often the partner used a long list of influence tactics on them. They also asked each partner to describe how much a small set of “relatively masculine” and “relatively feminine” traits are. (see Measurement Reliability and Factor Analysis). There are 235 couples included in the data analyses.
In Table 2 of Howard, Blumstein & Schwartz (1986), there are columns of standardized regression coefficients under columns for the predictor variables and the rows are the predicted (Y) variables, corresponding to what influence tactics a person did to his/her partner. For the first row regarding “Manipulation” as a tactic, there are numbers very close to zero and with no asterisks for Sex of Actor (-0.07), Sexual Orientation (-.018), Relative Masculinity (-.030), or Relative Femininity (.089), meaning that none of these predictors were reliably related to using Manipulation. Sex of Target, though has a coefficient of almost .20 (.191***) with three asterisks that the Note of the table explains means p < .001. The note also explains that the direction of this effect is that “positive regression coefficients indicate that … partners of men are perceived to use this tactic more than partners of women.” Using this “lesson” you should be able to infer that this means that men are perceived to have manipulation used against them more than women do, by about 20%. Further, because there is no reliable effect of sexual orientation (“-.018” is too close to zero to count) we can infer the former effect means that straight women are manipulating straight men more than would be expected by chance.
I typed in part of their Table 2 so as not to violate its copyright, WITH MY NOTES IN CAPS.
Table 2
Standardized Regression Coefficients for Regressions of Perceived Influence Tactic Use on Ssx and Sex Role Variables
Tactic | Sex of Actor | Sex of Target | Sexual Orientation | Relative Masculinity | Relative Femininity | |
Manipulation | -.071 | .191*** | -.018 | -.030 | .089 | |
Supplication | -.072 | .111* | -023 | -.119* | .119* |
Note. … IMPORTANTLY, THE AUTHORS EXPLAIN HOW TO INTERPRETATION THE DIRECTIONS (SIGNS) OF THE PREDICTOR VARIABLES BECAUSE THEY ARE CODED AS TWO NUMBERS AND IT IS ARBITRARY WHICH CATEGORY GOT THE HIGHER NUMBER. “Positive regression coefficients indicate that (a) mean are perceived to use the tactice more than women, (b) the are perceived to use the tactic more than are the partners of women, (c) the tactic occurs more in homosexual male and female couples than in heterosexual couples, and (d) those who are relatively more masculine or relatively more feminine are perceived to use the tactic more. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.”
Research citation:
Howard, J. A., Blumstein, P. & Schwarz, P. (1986). Sex, power and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 102-109.